Using Battleground States as a Natural Experiment to Test Theories of Voting*
نویسندگان
چکیده
We use variation produced by the Electoral College—the creation of battleground and non battleground states—to examine explanations for why people vote. We employ a longer time series (1980-2008) than previous research to gauge the effect of battleground status on state-level turnout. Our model includes (1) midterm elections, allowing us to directly compare the effect of battleground status with the broader increase in turnout associated with presidential elections, and (2) state fixed effects, which capture persistent state-level factors related to turnout. We find that the turnout boost from a presidential election is eight times the effect of being a battleground state. This suggests turnout is primarily linked to factors affecting the entire electorate, such as the social importance of presidential elections, rather than factors that influence just a portion of the country, such as intensive campaigning and mobilization efforts or a greater chance of casting a decisive vote. Understanding the level of and variation in electoral participation rates has absorbed the attention of scholars for decades. There is no consensus on which set of factors accounts best for the decision to vote, but researchers have offered a number of explanations. The economic theory of voting depicts voters weighing the costs of voting against the potential benefits of casting a decisive vote (Downs 1957). Other theoretical and empirical studies emphasize a sense of civic duty (Riker and Ordeshook 1968), social pressure (Gerber, Green, and Larimer 2008), mobilization efforts (Gerber and Green 2000; Rosenstone and Hansen 1993), and cost-benefit calculations pertaining to the convenience of voting (e.g., Brady and McNulty 2004; Dyck and Gimpel 2005) as affecting whether a person votes. In this paper, we exploit variation across states produced by the Electoral College system used in U.S. Presidential elections—the creation of “battleground” and “non battleground” states—to assess some of these explanations. Relative to their non battleground counterparts, battleground states receive an immense amount of attention from the parties because they are believed to be pivotal to the outcome of the election. The contrast between turnout in battleground and non battleground states gives us a measure of how much both the “pivotalness” of one’s vote and intensive campaigning and mobilization efforts in battleground states affect the chances an individual votes. Because our model includes observations from both presidential and midterm election years, we are also able to compare the size of this “battleground” effect to the effect a presidential election has on turnout across all states (whether a battleground or not), which we argue encapsulates the effects of those features of presidential elections which affect all citizens, such as the national focus on electing the most important political officer in the nation. In addition, this paper makes three more technical contributions to the literature on the effects of battleground status on turnout. First, we examine the effect of battleground status on state-level turnout 1 In 2008, for example, John McCain ($333 million) and Barack Obama ($730 million) combined to spend more than one billion dollars for the first time in history (Source: OpenSecrets.org), but “more than 98% of all campaign events and more than 98% of all campaign spending took place in only 15 states,” with the majority of these events and this spending taking place in only four states: Florida, Ohio, Pennsylvania, and Virginia (Source: FairVote.org). For theories and evidence on how the Electoral College influences campaign resource allocation strategies see Brams and Davis (1974); Colantoni, Levesque, and Ordeshook (1975); Bartels (1985); Cox and Munger (1989); James and Lawson (1999); Shaw (1999b, 2006); Reeves, Chen, and Nagano (2004); Strömberg (2008); Goux (2009); Shachar (2009). from 1980-2008 using statistical models that include both presidential and midterm election years. This allows us to estimate both the effect of being in a battleground state in a presidential election and the effect of the presidential election (compared to the midterm election) itself. Second, the length of our time series allows us to include state-level fixed effects in our model, thereby allowing us to control for any (measured or unmeasured) fixed, state-level characteristics that may be correlated with battleground status. Third, we also analyze the effect of changes in battleground status on change in turnout, which provides a way to separate the effect of status as a battleground state in the current election from any carryover effect from being a battleground state in the previous election. Our main finding is that the effect of presidential elections on turnout dwarfs the effect of being in a pivotal (battleground) state (the presidential election produces eight times the effect of battleground status). Moreover, for all the attention devoted to battleground states—from 1988-2008, on average, they received more than 80% of presidential campaign visits and advertising dollars (Shaw 1999a, 2006; Huang and Shaw forthcoming), turnout is only on average about one to two percentage points higher than in non battleground states. The large increase in turnout in presidential elections in all states, battleground and “spectator” states alike, suggests turnout levels are linked to factors that affect the entire electorate, such as perceptions about the importance of the presidency or the prominence of the national election as a topic of conversation, rather than factors that influence just a portion of the country, such as targeted campaigning and mobilization efforts or a greater chance of casting a decisive ballot. The remainder of this paper proceeds as follows. In the next section we discuss previous research on the effect of battleground status on voter turnout. We then present the model we use to test for the effects of battleground status on turnout, and discuss how it is an improvement over previous research. Next, we present our data and results, which show that the effect of status as a battleground state on turnout using an expanded set of elections is somewhat more modest than most recent research suggests, and is clearly much smaller than the common increase associated with the nationwide presidential election. We conclude with a discussion of the implications our findings have for understanding explanations for why people vote. 1. Previous Research: Battleground Status and Turnout A handful of recent studies have examined the effect of living in a battleground state on voter turnout. These studies employ different data, sometimes focusing on one year, other times on multiple years, and use different methodological approaches. While nearly all studies find the effect of battleground status to be less than about four percent, the specific estimates have been mixed. The results of these seven studies, including years studied and measures of battleground status and turnout, are summarized in Table 1. Three single-year studies find that battleground states have substantially higher turnout than non battleground states. McDonald (2008) observes that in 2008 turnout in the ten battleground states— classified as states that were visited by candidates from both campaigns in the last two weeks of the election—was four percentage points higher (65.9 versus 61.9%) than the remaining forty states. However, McDonald’s bivariate comparison does not control for any other factors that might also be related to turnout at the state level, such as whether a senatorial election was also being held in the state or baseline turnout levels in the state (e.g., turnout levels irrespective of the presidential race or battleground status). Bergan et al. (2005, see Table 4) examine the 2004 election and do control for these factors. Controlling for lagged battleground status and the average turnout in the state in the two previous midterm elections, they find that there was about a four or five percentage point turnout gap between battleground (measured using CNN state rankings) and non battleground states in 2004. Analyzing the 2000 election, Hill and McKee (2005, 715) find that the effect of battleground status (using Shaw’s [1999b, 2006] classification system) on turnout is slightly less than two percentage points. Although the 2 These ten battleground states are: Colorado, Florida, Indiana, Iowa, Missouri, Nevada, North Carolina, Ohio, Pennsylvania, and Virginia. 3 Bergan et al. (2005, Table 5) estimate another model predicting change in turnout that includes a lag for previous battleground status, which accounts for a change in battleground status from one election to the next. The coefficient for current battleground status in this regression is +3.67 (p<.001). 4 Shaw (1999b, 2006) identifies battleground states based upon information given to him by campaign consultants involved in planning the Electoral College strategies of the presidential candidates. He asked campaign strategists from each party to label every state as either (1) safe for one party (e.g., “base size of the battleground effect varies across these single-year studies, they tend to suggest that turnout is considerably higher in battleground states. Findings from multi-year studies are also somewhat mixed. In a study using the predicted closeness of the presidential race in the state—estimated by Campbell’s (1992) model for forecasting election results—to measure a state’s relative competitiveness, Shachar and Nalebuff (1999, see Table 6) estimate a structural model of participation and conclude “a 55:45 race should have a 1-percent higher participation rate than a 60:40 race” (532). In their analyses of presidential elections from 1992 to 2000, Holbrook and McClurg (2005) and Wolak (2006) report no difference in turnout rates between those states that are deemed to be battlegrounds and those that are not. Holbrook and McClurg use state-level turnout and campaign (presidential visits and media purchases and party transfers to the states) data and find that the parties focus on, and succeed at, mobilizing “core supporters” across states, but “find no clear evidence that the importance of campaigns for mobilization is dependent on competition in the states” (701, see Table 1). Examining the same sets of elections, but with American National Election Study (ANES) survey data, Wolak (see Table 3) also finds no effect of campaign battleground strategy (presidential advertising and campaign visits) on turnout intentions. Thus, two analyses of the same presidential elections (1992, 1996, and 2000) using different sets of data reach the same conclusion that living in a battleground state has no effect on turnout. In addition to considering only three elections, neither of these studies includes midterm election years or state fixed effects. In a recent article, Lipsitz (2009) argues that “we should expect to see greater E[lectoral] C[ollege] campaign strategy effects when elections are close than when there is a clear frontrunner” Republican”), (2) leaning toward one party (e.g., “lean Republican”), or (3) as a “battleground.” This creates five possible categories for each state. Hill and McKee (2005) consider those states that both parties assert are “battlegrounds” to be battleground states in their analysis, which they argue is appropriate given the fact that there was strong agreement between the parties in 2000. Other studies have used Shaw’s Electoral College strategy data to create a five-point measure of state competitiveness ranging from 0 (if both campaigns said a state was a safe state) to 4 (if both campaigns said a state was a battleground state) by summing Shaw’s scores for each party (where “base” equals 0, “lean” equals 1, and “battleground” equals 2) (see Gimpel, Kaufmann, and Pearson-Merkowitz 2007; Lipsitz 2009). 5 For a theoretical account, see Shachar (2009), who concludes that there is no direct effect of predicted closeness on turnout. Rather, his findings suggest that the effect of predicted closeness on turnout is only an indirect one through “marketing activities” (i.e., political advertising and other campaign efforts). (192). This leads her to hypothesize that, of the five presidential elections in her data set (1988-2004), the “effects [of battleground status] should be largest in 1988, 2000 and 2004 because of the competitiveness of those [national] elections” (193). According to this account, of the three elections analyzed by Holbrook and McClurg (2005) and Wolak (2006), only one (2000) is expected to exhibit substantial battleground state effects, which explains the null findings reported in those studies. Lipsitz finds a three percentage point turnout gap between battleground (defined by Shaw’s classification system, see footnote 4) and spectator states in 2000 and a six percentage point gap in 2004. Although Lipsitz’s important study controls for average turnout in the preceding two midterm elections, her model does not include midterm elections (as cases) and also does not include state fixed effects. In addition, the ex-post classification of races as “close” may not capture the anticipated closeness of the election. Further, if winning is the goal, concentrating campaign resources on a set of battleground states makes strategic sense even when the final outcome is less competitive. 1.1 Extending Previous Work on the Effects of Battleground Status on Turnout We seek to build on previous studies in three ways. First, we include midterm elections as part of our data set so that we can make a direct comparison between the effect of being a battleground state in a presidential election and the effect of the presidential election itself. The relative magnitude of these effects can offer insight into what voters are thinking when they decide whether to vote. Second, we employ a time series (1980 to 2008) that is long enough to provide sufficient degrees of freedom to include state fixed effects. The inclusion of state-level fixed effects ensures that the coefficient on our indicator of battleground status can be interpreted as the effect of being a battleground state on turnout, rather than any other state-level factors that may be correlated with both battleground status and turnout. Third, previous work attributes the effect of being a battleground state on turnout to being a battleground state in that election, which may produce misleading estimates because not many states move in and out of the “battlefield” (a problem magnified by a lack of state fixed effects). Thus, it could be the case that a longstanding history as a battleground state is what a “battleground” coefficient is estimating. If battleground status matters more when a state first enters the battlefield (or does so after a hiatus), then the effect of battleground status observed in previous work may overestimate the effect of continued battleground status and underestimate the effect of becoming a battleground. Indeed, there is some suggestive evidence that this could be the case. As McDonald notes for the 2008 election, New entrants to the Electoral College battleground in 2008 experienced the largest turnout increases from 2004. Thus Indiana’s turnout rate rose 4.5 percentage points, from 54.8% to 59.3%; North Carolina improved 8.0 percentage points, from 57.8% to 65.8%; and Virginia increased 6.8 percentage points, from 60.6% to 67.4%. States that left the battlefield, such as Maine, Minnesota, Oregon, Washington and Wisconsin, experienced turnout declines or failed to keep pace with the national increase. Other states that remained in play essentially had the same levels of turnout (2008, 2). Thus, we believe a potentially more informative test of whether battleground status at time t affects turnout at time t is to examine changes in battleground status from time t-1. 2. Modeling the Effect of Battleground Status on State-level Turnout To concretely illustrate the advantages of an approach employing (1) midterm election years, (2) state fixed effects, and (3) changes in battleground status, we outline our modeling strategy by first detailing how it deviates from previous work. Consider first a model of the single-year case in which state-level turnout (TOi, the i subscript denotes states) is the dependent variable, and the independent variable is whether a state is considered a battleground (BGi = 1) or not (BGi = 0). In this instance, we estimate the effect of BG on TO using the following model: (1) TOi = B0 + B1BGi + Ui, where Ui represents unobserved error in the model. In (1), the effect of BG is biased if the disturbance term, Ui, contains omitted determinants of TOi that are correlated with battleground status. Studies of the effect of battleground status on turnout typically account for this concern by including some measure of the baseline turnout rate (BTi) of a state and a vector (Zi) of other control variables, such as whether a senatorial or gubernatorial election is also being held in the state during that year, because these factors are also likely to affect TO in the presidential election. With these controls, we can rewrite (1) as: (2) TOi = B0 + B1BGi + B2BTi + γZi + Ui. However, the inclusion of BT and Z (e.g., senatorial and gubernatorial election indicators) does not solve the problem of being able to identify the direct effect of BG on TO because what determines BG may be correlated with omitted variables across states (e.g., the presence of particularly close House or state level elections, or more developed state parties). This is the case even in a panel design that only examines presidential election years: (3) TOi,t = B0 + B1BGi,t + B2BTi,t + γZi,t + τTt + Ui,t, where the new subscript t denotes time, and T is a vector of year-specific intercepts. In this panel design, there is still uncertainty surrounding the causal relationship between BG and TO if there are omitted factors (in U) that are correlated with both BG and TO. Moreover, even if model (3) were perfectly specified (i.e., U has a mean of 0), it does not allow us to estimate the effect of having a presidential election relative to not having one because midterm elections are not included. In order to isolate the effects of the presidential election and being a battleground state in a presidential election, we include in our model both midterm and presidential elections. Thus, we rewrite (3) as: (4) TOi,t = B0 + B1BGi,t + B2Pt + γZi,t + τTt + ψSi + Ui,t, where the new terms are P and S. P is a dichotomous indicator for whether the current election is a presidential or midterm election that does not vary across states. S is a vector of state indicators, used to estimate state fixed effects. These fixed effects account for any time-invariant characteristics that affect state-level turnout. We note that even with the inclusion of the state fixed effects, this model does not permit us to estimate the effect of changes in battleground status on turnout. In order to do so, we employ two approaches: (1) we introduce one and two election lags for battleground status and (2) we regress change in turnout on changes in battleground status. (We describe each of these approaches in greater detail below.) 3. Data and Results We use the variation in the choice environment across battleground and non battleground states to test theories of voting based on an assumption that any turnout effects of battleground status stem from the fact that people in these states are (quasi-randomly) assigned to receive more intensive campaign efforts and have a greater likelihood that their votes are decisive for the election outcome. In contrast, any common effect of a presidential election on turnout presumably is a product of factors that affect the entire electorate. For this study to approximate a “natural experiment,” it must be the case that battleground states are not created by some underlying process or mechanism that is related to state-level turnout. We offer a number of observations in defense of this assumption here. We begin by noting that, given that we include state fixed effects in our model (see above), any underlying source of variation that affects both status as a battleground state and voter turnout must change over time (i.e., it cannot be a stable characteristic). One possible source of change is that new migrants may cause a state to become more competitive because the migrants’ ideological dispositions may mitigate the dominant ideology present in the state. While there are certainly fluctuations in state population size, they do not appear to be linked to changes in state competitiveness. For instance, in presidential elections from 1980-2008, the state-level change in the voting age population (VAP) is correlated with the two-party margin of victory in the state at r = -.08 and changes in the two-party margin of victory in the state at r = -.03. Thus, there does not appear to be any strong evidence that changes in the adult population in a state from one presidential election to the next make the state substantially more competitive. Moreover, changes in a state’s VAP are only weakly correlated with changes in turnout in the state from one presidential election to the next at r = .04. Another possibility is that presidential candidates change the competitive environment in their home states. From 1980 to 2008 five presidential home states move into (two states) or out of (three states) battleground status based on our measure of state pivotalness (described below). In addition, from 1980 to 2008 presidential home states are weakly and positively correlated with a state’s two-party margin of victory (r = .06), suggesting that on average president’s make their home state’s less competitive. However, changes in the two-party margin of victory are almost completely uncorrelated with the home state of presidential candidates (r = .01), suggesting presidential candidates do not substantially change the competitive environments of their home states. We find similarly weak 6 CA (1984 Reagan) and TX (1988 Bush) become pivotal states, while CA (1980 Reagan), TX (1992 Bush), and AZ (2008 McCain) are no longer pivotal. correlations between an indicator for presidential candidate home state and both turnout (r = -.07, 19802008) and changes in turnout (r = -.002, 1984-2008). 3.1 Data Our time series extends from 1980-2008 and includes 750 observations (50 states x 15 elections [7 midterm, 8 presidential]). We specify state-level voter turnout as percent turnout (0-100) among the voting eligible population (VEP). The VEP data were collected and made available by Professor Michael P. McDonald. For battleground status we cannot rely solely on Shaw’s classification of battleground states, which is based on his reports from the campaigns, because our time series includes two elections (1980 and 1984) that Shaw does not classify. Instead, we create a measure of state “battlegroundness” (or, more accurately, whether the state was pivotal to the election outcome) based on actual election outcomes, and use Shaw’s categorization as a robustness check for the years it is available (1988-2008, see Shaw 1999b, 2006 and Huang and Shaw forthcoming). We measure the pivotalness of the state to the overall electoral outcome based on the actual election outcome observed in the state. Specifically, for each presidential election year, we arrayed the states by the Democratic share of the two-party vote and then cumulatively summed the Electoral College votes by state in this order. We then subtracted that number from 270 (the number of Electoral College votes needed in order to be declared the victor) and took the absolute value of that figure. This gives us an indicator of how likely it was that the state was pivotal in determining the outcome of the election. We 7 If we omit candidate home states from the analyses presented below, the substantive conclusions we draw do not change. These results are available upon request. 8 In 1982, Louisiana did not hold congressional elections in November because all of the races were settled earlier in the election season during Louisiana’s open primary. Therefore, there is no recorded turnout for Louisiana in the 1982 general election and we lose this observation from our data set, making our total number of observations 749. 9 The data can be downloaded from: http://elections.gmu.edu/Turnout.html (McDonald 2009). The data only extend back to 1980. Therefore, when we include lagged turnout in some of the model specifications presented below (Appendix Table A3), we use the voting age population (VAP) as our denominator for calculating state-level voter turnout because this allows us to calculate turnout rates for years prior to 1980. classified any state that was within 100 electoral votes of 270 as a pivotal state (i.e., battleground). See Appendix Table A1 for a list of pivotal states from 1972-2008 based on this measure. Most people are aware of how pivotal their vote is to the election outcome (see Blais 2000, chapter 3). We confirm that our measure also correlates with the distribution of campaign activity in presidential elections empirically. Table 2 displays presidential advertising and campaign visit data from 1988-2008 (see Shaw 1999a, 2006; Huang and Shaw forthcoming) in battleground (BG) and non battleground (NBG) states based on both our measure (left hand side of the table) and Shaw’s (right hand side of the table). The table highlights the discrepancy between BG and NBG states in both the number of visits and the amount of money spent on advertising: battleground states always receive substantially more of both. Thus, our indicator appears to measure aspects of the presidential campaign linked to battleground states. As a further validity and reliability check of our pivotal state measure, we compare our measure to Shaw’s classification system (described in footnote 4 and used in previous work [Gimpel, Kaufmann, and Pearson-Merkowitz 2007; Hill and McKee 2005; Lipsitz 2009]). Specifically, if either party reported to Shaw that a given state was a “battleground” we give it a score of one; if neither party identified a given state as a battleground we give it a score of zero. Our measure of pivotal states is highly correlated with this measure (r=0.56, p<.001). Given that Shaw’s measure indicates larger discrepancies in campaign activity between BG and NBG states than our measure (see the right hand side of Table 2), we 10 The results we present below are robust to different cutpoints—75 and 125 electoral votes. These results are available upon request. 11 We are unaware of any direct evidence on the question of whether residents of battleground states perceive their vote to be more pivotal than residents of non battleground states. 12 Presidential advertising is measured in terms of audience exposure, or gross ratings points (GRPs). “Roughly speaking, one hundred GRPs represent 100% of voters in a [media] market seeing an advertisement once. [The] statewide GRPs are calculated by multiplying the number of GRPs bought in a market by the percentage of the state’s eligible voters in that market, repeating the procedure for all markets in a state, and then summing these numbers” (Shaw 1999a, 348-9). conduct robustness checks of our models using his measure to ensure that our pivotal state measure does not lead us to underestimate the effect of battleground status on turnout. As a first look at the data, Figure 1 plots turnout (as a % of VEP) in the presidential election (yaxis) by turnout (as a % of VEP) in the previous midterm election (x-axis) for the six most recent presidential elections (1988-2008). In each plot, the states are marked as either battleground (B) or safe (S), as defined by our pivotal state measure, and a linear regression line is shown for each set of states (solid for B; dashed for S). Thus, the plots show the difference between battleground and safe states in presidential election turnout relative to midterm election turnout without statistical controls. Two aspects of Figure 1 are worthy of note. First, there appears to be a clear time trend: there is more of a gap (i.e., distance between the regression lines) between battleground and safe states in 2004 and, especially, 2008 than for any other year. Second, the differences between battleground and safe states, even in 2004 and 2008, pale in comparison to the differences between midterm and presidential elections. As illustrated by the fact that almost all cases are substantially above the 45 degree line, the average turnout difference between midterm and presidential elections from 1982-2008 is nearly 16 percentage points. Even in 2004 and 2008, the difference in turnout between the previous midterm election and current presidential election—a difference of approximately 20 percentage points in both years (see Table 3 below)—is far greater than the difference in turnout between battleground and safe states in those years (4 percentage points in 2004; 6 in 2008). Thus, Figure 1 provides some suggestive evidence that (1) battleground status has some impact on turnout in presidential elections, but that (2) this 13 These results are presented in the Appendix (Tables A2 and A4). In general, our results are robust to using Shaw’s measure (and the time period 1988-2008). The only discrepancy involves the analysis of changes in battleground status on changes in turnout (see Appendix Table A4 and footnote 22). However, these differences no change the substantive conclusions we make. 14 1984 looks very similar to 1988, so we omit it to save space. We do not have an estimate of the VEP in 1978, so we do not create a similar plot for 1980. Appendix Figure A1 shows plots using Shaw’s categorization of battleground states from 1988-2008. 15 During this period, average turnout in presidential elections was 58.07%; average turnout in midterm elections was 42.43%. 16 Average turnout in battleground states was 67.22% in 2008 and 64.58% in 2004; average turnout in non battleground states was 61.49% in 2008 and 60.82% in 2004. effect is much smaller than the effect of the presidential election on all states (battleground and safe alike). In the next section, we test these findings more rigorously. 3.2 The Effect of Pivotal State Status on Turnout Beyond the variables discussed above, our model includes three measures to capture whether other high profile races were occurring in the state in a given year. Specifically, we include indicators for whether a (1) Senate or (2) gubernatorial election was held in a state and a measure (3) of the proportion of House races that were decided by 10 points or less in a state, where 0 indicates no competitive House races and 1 indicates that all House races were competitive in that state-year. These three measures are interacted with the presidential and midterm election year indicators because the effect of having other high profile races may be larger in midterm election years. For each measure for ease of interpretation, we include both interaction terms—(1) presidential year with the measure and (2) midterm year with the measure—rather than the component term and one interaction term. Table 3 presents the summary statistics for all the variables used in the analyses that follow. To test for the effect of status as a pivotal (or, battleground) state on voter turnout, we estimate the following model: (A) Turnouti,t = b + b1Pivotal Statei,t + b2Presidential Electiont + b3Proportion House Races Competitivei,t*Presidential Electiont + b4Proportion House Races Competitivei,t*Midterm Electiont + b5 Senatorial Electioni,t*Presidential Electiont + b6Senatorial Electioni,t*Midterm Electiont + b7Gubernatorial Electioni,t*Presidential Electiont + b8Gubernatorial Electioni,t*Midterm Electiont + τTt + ψSi + ei,t, where S is a vector of state fixed effects, T is a vector of year fixed effects, and e is the error term. By using this modeling strategy, we are able to estimate the effect of being a pivotal state in a presidential election, holding constant the effect of the presidential election itself and any state-level characteristics that affect turnout. Table 4 presents the results of our model (A) in column (1), where midterm elections are given a score of 0 on our pivotal state measure, and column (5), where midterm elections are given the score of 17 We thank Professor Gary C. Jacobson for generously sharing his U.S. House election data with us. the previous presidential election on our pivotal state measure. The other columns introduce different lag structures for the main independent variable as a way of testing whether the effect of battleground status carries over from one presidential election to the next. Specifically, columns (2) and (6) only include the previous presidential election’s pivotal state score, columns (3) and (7) include both the current and previous presidential election’s pivotal state scores, and columns (4) and (8) add a second lag to the column (3) and (7) model specification. Two aspects of the results are particularly striking. First, the effect of the presidential election year indicator is consistently around 16 to 17 percentage points. This result suggests the nationwide social phenomenon of a presidential election that occurs every four years has a substantial impact on turnout in and of itself. Second, the effect of being in a pivotal state pales in comparison to that of the presidential election. Our results indicate that between 1980 and 2008, being in a pivotal state is associated with an increase of, at most, about two percentage points in state-level turnout (see column [1]). When we allow for a carryover effect into midterm elections, this effect is only about one percentage point (see column [5]). Given the intensive campaigning that occurs in these “pivotal” states (see Table 2), it is somewhat surprising that we do not find a more substantial effect. These results suggest that turnout is much more strongly affected by factors that affect the entire electorate, such as the social importance of presidential elections, than factors that influence just a portion of the country, such as intensive campaigning and mobilization efforts or the likelihood of casting a decisive vote. Second, the coefficients for lagged battleground status indicate that the effect of being a pivotal state on turnout carries over from the previous election. Columns (3) and (4) suggest that the effect of current status as a pivotal state on turnout is about 1.5 percentage points (compared to two percentage points without the lags). Although the effects of previous battleground status are small—less than one 18 Our approach to measuring battleground status allows us to calculate lagged battleground status for 1972 and 1976. percentage point—they are statistically significant. Below, we present an alternative model specification that allows us to disentangle the effects of remaining a battleground state from the effects of either becoming or ceasing to be a battleground state. Prior to doing so, we discuss several other noteworthy results from Table 4. The findings for the measures of other high profile races in the state identified in Table 4 are also important. In particular, the interaction term for Midterm year * Senate election indicates that, ceteris paribus, turnout is a little more than two percentages points higher in states that have a Senate election in midterm elections (the insignificant coefficient on the Presidential year * Senate election variable indicates that there is no difference in presidential elections). Gubernatorial elections appear to have even more of an effect on turnout in midterm elections (but, again, no effect in presidential elections): the interaction term Midterm year * Gubernatorial election indicates that turnout is nearly five percentage points higher in states that hold gubernatorial elections in midterm election years. The fact that Senate and gubernatorial elections only increase turnout in midterm elections suggests that there is a ceiling, of sorts, on the size of the electorate. That is, there is a set of people who vote without fail (or nearly so) and another set of people who (almost) never vote. Between these two extremes is a third set of people who have the potential to be mobilized to vote depending on the circumstances of the election. In presidential elections, most of these people already vote, hence the insignificant coefficients of Senate and gubernatorial elections interacted with presidential election year. However, the presence of a Senate or gubernatorial election in midterm election years attracts a segment of the group of people who tend to only vote if there is an important and/or high profile election. We also note that, although neither coefficient is statistically significant, the coefficient on the Presidential year * 19 To show that our findings are not entirely driven by the inclusion of midterm years, Appendix Table A3 limits our analysis to presidential election years (i.e., midterm elections are no longer included in the model) and includes a measure of lagged turnout (in column [3]). This table shows that whether the effect of pivotal state status on turnout is estimated in a model that includes lagged turnout (column [3]) or lagged pivotal state status (Appendix Table A3, column [4] and all of Table 4) has little bearing on the coefficient for current pivotal state status. 20 This effect is comparable to that found by Smith (2001, Table 2). Competitive House race interaction is larger than the coefficient on the Midterm year * Competitive House race term. 3.3 The Effect of Changes in Pivotal State Status on Change in Turnout As noted above, our longer time series permits a more detailed analysis of the effect of battleground status on turnout because there are more changes in which states are considered pivotal. We examine whether becoming a pivotal state results in a larger turnout boost than being a pivotal state in successive elections in Table 5 (all analysis shown in Table 5 includes only presidential election years). We regress change in turnout on three indicators of pivotal state status: (1) whether the state became pivotal since the last presidential election (i.e., new “treatment”); (2) whether the state was pivotal in the previous presidential election but no longer is in the current presidential election (i.e., old “treatment”); and (3) whether the state was pivotal in the previous presidential election and still is in the current presidential election (i.e., new and old “treatment”). The inclusion of these three indicators makes the omitted reference category states that were not battlegrounds in the previous presidential election and remain safe in the current presidential election. Thus, the model we estimate is: (B) ΔTurnouti,t-t-1 = b + b1New Treatmenti,t + b2Old Treatmenti,t + b3New and Old Treatmenti,t + τTt + ψSi + ei,t. We also include controls for changes in Senate and gubernatorial election status in the same manner as the changes in pivotal state status variables. In Table 5, we present the results of this model using VEP in columns (1) and (2) and, so that our series can include 1980 (which requires a lagged value of turnout in 1976; see footnote 9), Voting Age Population (VAP) in columns (7) and (8). The other columns in Table 5 present results for slight modifications to model (B). In particular, columns (3) and (4) [VEP] and (9) and (10) [VAP] omit the new and old treatment indicator so that the reference category is all states that do not change status (either because they remain pivotal states or remain not pivotal states). Columns (5) and (6) [VEP] and (11) and (12) combine the four change in pivotal state status measures into one variable so that new treatment states are equal to 1, old treatment states are equal to -1, and both (a) new and old treatment states and (b) states that are not pivotal in successive elections are equal to 0. State fixed effects are included in the odd numbered columns and excluded from the models in the even numbered columns. The analysis of change in turnout as a function of changes in battleground status presented in column 1 shows that states that become pivotal (i.e., are new to the battlefield) experience somewhat larger turnout increases than those that maintain their pivotal state status from one election to the next. Newly pivotal states experience an increase in turnout of about 1.2 percentage points (compared to those states that are never pivotal); turnout in states that maintain their pivotal status increases by about 65% as much (around .8 percentage points when compared to those states that are never pivotal). Thus, while there is a boost in turnout for being a pivotal state a second time, it is not quite as much as the increase the state experiences when it initially becomes a battleground. These results are robust to the inclusion of state fixed effects and the use of VAP (which allows the inclusion of 1980). Also of note is that compared to states that are not pivotal in successive elections, there appears to be a slight persistence (or, habit) effect caused by states that were part of the battlefield in the previous presidential election but no longer are in the current election (see columns [1] and [2] and [7] and [8]), although this “old treatment” coefficient never reaches statistical significance. 4. Discussion and Conclusion The perception that one’s vote is pivotal is thought to play an important role in the voter’s decision-making calculus (Downs 1957; Riker and Ordeshook 1968). Yet, people in the most pivotal states do not appear to turn out at a substantially higher rate than those in “safe” states—no more than about two percentage points on average according to our analysis of elections from 1980-2008 (see Table 4). This is the case despite the fact that in addition to being pivotal to the election outcome, campaigns 21 A joint hypothesis test reveals that these coefficients are not statistically distinguishable from one another (p = .43). 22 We note that the results presented in Table 5 are the one instance in which using Shaw’s categorization of battleground states seems to matter. In particular, using Shaw’s measure, any effect of changing battleground status on change in turnout appears to be driven by states that leave the battlefield having lower turnout. devote massive amounts of resources to stimulate the electorate in battleground states. This finding raises the question of why battleground status does not have more of an effect on turnout, especially when we consider that the mere presence of a presidential election boosts turnout by 16 percentage points, or about eight times as much. One explanation, which finds some support in Table 5, is that the impact on voter turnout among states that were battlegrounds in the previous election is a bit less than among formerly safe states that move into the battlefield (although this difference is not statistically significant). Thus, future work aimed at identifying how changes in battleground status from one election to the next affect political behaviors and attitudes may be fruitful, particularly in identifying which stimuli (e.g., having voted in the past [Gerber, Green, and Shachar 2003], registration [Cain and McCue 1985], political communication [Huber and Arceneaux 2007], etc.) persist over time. We qualify our conclusions with a number of caveats. First, our finding that the effect of battleground status is only (in relative terms) about two percentage points does not mean that candidate and party effort is wasted on battleground states. Indeed, in some instances a small boost in turnout for one party or the other could mean the difference between victory and defeat. Instead, we emphasize that the effects of these efforts are quite modest compared with the broader effects of holding a presidential election. Second, we note that there does appear to be some variation over time in the effect of battleground status on turnout. Lipsitz (2009) attributes some of this variation over time to the overall competitiveness of the election. However, she finds larger effects on turnout for battleground status in 2004 (six percentage points), which turned out to be a less competitive election than 2000 (where she finds a three percentage point effect). Thus, overall competitiveness cannot be the sole explanation. Future research may want to consider whether changes in what states become battlegrounds and/or resource differences (that perhaps vary by party, see Goux 2009) account for such across-year differences. Another possibility is that there is a greater turnout gap between battleground and safe states in recent years (especially 2004 and 2008 as indicated by our scatter plots in Figure 1) because voter mobilization efforts, including more targeted advertising, have become more precise and effective (see Bergan et al. 2005). Indeed, it is difficult to believe that changes in the importance of pivotalness to voters over time are responsible for greater turnout disparities between battleground and non battleground states in 2004 and 2008. Rather, it is more likely the case that more sophisticated (i.e., targeted) campaigning at least partially explains the turnout differences between battleground and non battleground states. With this possibility in mind, we conducted a supplementary analysis in which we included presidential campaign TV advertising (in GRPs, see footnote 12) as an independent variable in our Table 4, column (1) model specification. If patterns of TV advertising between battleground and non battleground states have changed over time (e.g., became more targeted at battleground states), it is likely that the targeting of other campaign activities has also changed in a similar manner, in which case advertising GRPs will also serve as a proxy for those activities. Given that we only have advertising data beginning in 1988, the supplementary analysis is only conducted for the years 1988-2008. In order to provide a more direct comparison to our earlier results, Table 6, column (1) replicates the Table 4, column (1) specification for this shorter time period. The coefficients for the pivotal state indicator are nearly identical—2.152 in Table 6 compared to 2.020 in Table 4. Column (2) then presents the results when we allow the effect of pivotal state status to vary by presidential election year. While all the interaction terms (pivotal state status with each presidential election year) are positive, only those for 2008 and 2004 are statistically different from zero. Therefore, there is some indication that there was more of an effect of battleground status on turnout in the two most recent presidential elections. In column (3), we add the advertising data, allowing its effect to vary by presidential election year as we did battleground status in column (2). Accounting for presidential campaign advertising reduces the size of the pivotal state indicator by about 40%, suggesting that TV advertising (and other campaign activities correlated with it) does account for a sizable portion of the battleground effect. Moreover, only the advertising coefficients for 2008 and 2004 are statistically significant, suggesting that advertising and 23 Tests of the equality of the coefficients indicate that pivotal state status in 2008 and 2004 are not distinguishable from one another (p = .21), but that they are distinguishable from each of the other pivotal state/presidential election year interactions (p < .05). correlated activities in these two years may primarily account for this portion of the effect. Finally, in column (4) we allow both the effect of pivotal state status and presidential campaign advertising to vary by presidential election year. Compared to column (2), the size of the coefficients for pivotal state in 2008 and 2004 are reduced by about 25 and 55%, respectively. This serves as some indication that campaign activities are responsible for some portion of the “battleground effect.” Indeed, if we it were possible to measure all campaign activities (e.g., party transfers to the states, door-to-door canvassing, etc.), the “pure” pivotal state effect might approach zero. Therefore, while it is difficult to differentiate increases in pivotalness from intensive campaigning and mobilization efforts in battleground states, this supplementary analysis appears to suggest that party efforts are in part responsible for any observed turnout effects. Moreover, regardless of how much of the battleground effect originates in campaigns, the aggregate battleground effect is still much less than the effect of factors that affect the entire electorate during a presidential election, captured by our presidential election indicator, which indicates a boost in turnout of at least 17 percentages points in Table 6. Our analysis cannot determine what factors related to presidential elections are not also present in midterm elections. Possibilities range from differences in peoples’ sense of civic duty (Riker and Ordeshook 1968), factors related to social pressure (Gerber, Green, and Larimer 2008), registration and mobilization efforts that occur in all states during presidential elections (Gerber and Green 2000; Rosenstone and Hansen 1993), habit (Gerber, Green, and Shachar 2003), or perhaps simply the importance of the presidency (compared to other offices) to the American electorate. However, based on the present analysis, it seems clear that beliefs about the instrumental value of one’s vote cannot explain these differences. Put another way, in our data, the average difference between turnout in midterm and presidential elections exceeds 14 percentage points in states like Utah (17.9%), Massachusetts (14.3%), and Mississippi (23.4%). During the time period we examine these states never come close to being 24 Tests of the equality of the coefficients indicate that only the coefficient for advertising in 2004 is distinguishable from advertising in any other years (p < .05). The coefficient for 2004 is larger than the coefficients for all other years except for 1988 (p = .71). 25 From 1980-2008, there were three instances in which turnout in a presidential election was actually lower than turnout in the previous midterm election: Alaska (1984) and Hawaii (1996 & 2000). pivotal, making it highly unlikely that these differences can be explained by expectations about the value of one’s vote to the election outcome. Last, we note that our measure of pivotalness (i.e., the instrumental value of one’s vote) is based on objective election outcomes, but it is likely that perceived pivotalness is what is of direct relevance to prospective voters. If it is the case that residents of non battleground states overestimate their probability of being pivotal more so than residents of battleground states, then we have underestimated the effect of pivotalness on turnout. While it seems unlikely that the perceived pivotalness among the electorate would be skewed in this direction, we encourage future survey research that attempts to measure perceptions of pivotalness for residents of battleground and non battleground states to see if this assumption is valid. Our principal concern in this paper was to estimate the effect of battleground status on turnout and compare the magnitude of this effect to the effect of simply holding a presidential election. We employed a new modeling strategy that more precisely captures this effect than previous work. Our finding that the boost in voter turnout associated with presidential elections dwarfs the effect of battleground status on voter turnout suggests that the reason for the observed surge in turnout in presidential election years is likely the result of universally held beliefs about the importance of the office or other factors that affect the entire electorate, rather than beliefs about the instrumentality of one’s vote or more focused campaign efforts in battleground states. Although precisely what norms are primarily responsible for the large effects of presidential elections remains to be seen, it is clear that such norms trump any “extra” motivation to vote—in the form of advertising and campaign visits by the parties and/or an increased sense that one’s vote is pivotal to the election outcome—induced by residency in a battleground state. References Bartels, Larry M. 1985. “Resource Allocation in a Presidential Campaign.” Journal of Politics 47 (3): 928-936. Bergan, Daniel E., Alan S. Gerber, Donald P. Green, and Costas Panagopoulos. 2005. “Grassroots Mobilization and Voter Turnout in 2004.” Public Opinion Quarterly 69 (5): 760-777. Blais, André. 2000. To Vote or Not To Vote? The Merits and Limits of Rational Choice Theory. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press. Brady, Henry. E., and John E. McNulty. 2004. “The Costs of Voting: Evidence from a Natural Experiment.” Presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Political Methodology, Palo Alto, CA. Brams, Steven J., and Morton D. Davis. 1974. “The 3/2’s Rule in Presidential Campaigning.” American Political Science Review 68 (1): 113-134. Campbell, James E. 1992. “Forecasting the Presidential Vote in the States.” American Journal of Political Science 36 (2): 386-407. Colantoni, Claude S., Terrence J. Levesque, and Peter C. Ordeshook. 1975. “Campaign Resource Allocations under the Electoral College.” American Political Science Review 69 (1): 141-154. Cox, Gary W., and Michael C. Munger. 1989. “Closeness, Expenditures, and Turnout in the 1982 U.S. House Elections.” American Political Science Review 83 (1): 217-231. Downs, Anthony. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper & Row. Dyck, Joshua. J., and James G. Gimpel. 2005. “Distance, Turnout, and the Convenience of Voting.” Social Science Quarterly 86 (3): 531-548. FairVote.org. 2008. “2008’s Shrinking Battleground and Its Start Impact on Campaign Activity.” December 4. http://fairvote.org/?page=27&pressmode;=showspecific&showarticle;=230 (July 9, 2009). Gerber, Alan S., and Donald P. Green. 2000. “The Effect of a Nonpartisan Get-Out-The-Vote Drive: An Experimental Study of Leafletting. Journal of Politics 62 (3): 846-57. Gerber, Alan S., Donald P. Green, and Ron Shachar. 2003. “Voting May Be Habit-Forming: Evidence from a Randomized Field Experiment.” American Journal of Political Science 47 (3): 540-550. Gerber, Alan S., Donald P. Green, and Christopher W. Larimer. 2008. “Social Pressure and Voter Turnout: Evidence from a Large-Scale Field Experiment.” American Political Science Review 102 (1): 33-48. Gimpel, James G., Karen M. Kaufmann, and Shanna Pearson-Merkowitz. 2007. “Battleground States versus Blackout States: The Behavioral Implications of Modern Presidential Campaigns.” Journal of Politics 69 (3): 786-797. Goux, Darshan. 2009. “Conceptualizing the Battleground State: The Basics.” University of California, Berkeley. Typescript. Hill, David, and Seth C. McKee. 2005. “The Electoral College, Mobilization, and Turnout in the 2000 Presidential Election.” American Politics Research 33 (5): 700-725. Huang, Taofang, and Daron Shaw. N.d. “Beyond the Battlegrounds? Electoral College Strategies in the 2008 Presidential Election.” Journal of Political Marketing. Forthcoming. Holbrook, Thomas M., and Scott D. McClurg. 2005. “The Mobilization of Core Supporters: Campaigns, Turnout, and Electoral Composition in United States Presidential Elections.” American Journal of Political Science 49 (4): 689-703. James, Scott C. and Brian L. Lawson. 1999. “The Political Economy of Voting Rights Enforcement in America’s Gilded Age: Electoral College Competition, Partisan Commitment, and the Federal Election Law.” American Political Science Review 93 (1): 115-131. Lipsitz, Keena. 2009. “The Consequences of Battleground and ‘Spectator’ State Residency for Political Participation.” Political Behavior 31 (2): 187-209. McDonald, Michael P. 2008. “The Return of the Voter: Voter Turnout in the 2008 Presidential Election.” The Forum 6 (4): article 4. McDonald, Michael P. 2002. “The Turnout Rate Among Eligible Voters for U.S. States, 19802000.” State Politics and Policy Quarterly 2 (2): 199-212. McDonald, Michael P. 2009. “Turnout 1980-2008.” http://elections.gmu.edu/Turnout.html (July 13, 2009). McDonald, Michael P., and Samuel Popkin. 2001. “The Myth of the Vanishing Voter.”American Political Science Review 95 (4): 963-974. OpenSecrets.org. 2008. “Banking on Becoming President.” http://www.opensecrets.org/pres08/index.php (July 9, 2009). Reeves, Andrew, Lanhee Chen, and Tiffany Nagano. “A Reassessment of ‘The Methods behind the Madness: Presidential Electoral College Strategies, 1988-1996’.” Journal of Politics 66 (2): 616-620. Riker, William H., and Peter C. Ordeshook. 1968. “A Theory of the Calculus of Voting.” American Political Science Review 62 (1): 25-42. Rosenstone, Steven J., and John Mark Hansen. 1993. Mobilization, Participation, and Democracy in America. New York: MacMillian Publishing Company. Shachar, Ron. 2009. “The Political Participation Puzzle and Marketing.” Journal of Marketing Research. Forthcoming. Shachar, Ron, and Barry Nalebuff. 1999. “Follow the Leader: Theory and Evidence on Political Participation.” American Economic Review 89 (3): 525-547. Shaw, Daron. 1999a. “The Effect of TV Ads and Candidate Appearances on Statewide Presidential Votes, 1988-96.” American Political Science Review 93 (2): 345-361. Shaw, Daron. 1999b. “The Methods behind the Madness: Presidential Electoral College Strategies, 1988-1996.” Journal of Politics 61 (4): 893-913. Shaw, Daron. 2006. The Race to 270. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Smith, Mark A. 2001. “The Contingent Effects of Ballot Initiatives and Candidate Races on Turnout.” American Journal of Political Science 45 (3): 700-706. Strömberg, David. 2008. “How the Electoral College Influences Campaigns and Policy: The Probability of Being Florida.” American Economic Review 98 (3): 769-807. Wolak, Jennifer. 2006. “The Consequences of Presidential Battleground Strategies for Citizen Engagement.” Political Research Quarterly 59 (3): 353-361. Table A1. List of Pivotal States, by Year State 1972 1976 198
منابع مشابه
Voter Turnout Among College Students: New Data and a Rethinking of Traditional Theories
Objectives. Traditional theories of turnout are of limited applicability to college students: the concepts and measures associated with these theories were not designed with students in mind, and factors not considered by the traditional theories are relevant. We offer a new theoretical perspective for understanding college student turnout and test it using a new data set. Methods. We conducted...
متن کاملLiving in a Battleground: Presidential Campaigns and Fundamental Predictors of Vote Choice
Little evidence links the strategic decisions of campaigns to individual-level voting behavior. Yet for campaigns to matter in the way that experts argue, exposure to campaigns must also matter so there should be observable differences in the structure of vote choice between battleground and non-battleground states. Combining presidential campaign data with the Senate Election Study, we show th...
متن کاملThe Evaluation of Environmental Quality criteria in Urban Design Using Citizens' Cognitive Characteristics; (Case Study: Tehran Neighborhoods)
The problem which has been the focus of city constructors and architectures since the beginning of citizenship life is the issue of the nature of environmental quality. This study, with a review of explaining paradigms of environmental quality and by reliance on basic works, has assumed the question of nature of environmental quality an interdisciplinary concept (civic construction, sociology, ...
متن کاملActing on the Intent to Vote : A Voter Turnout Experiment
Theories of voter turnout have focused almost exclusively on the costs and benefits of voting, even though one potentially important aspect of turnout is what takes place after a citizen has decided she intends to vote but before she arrives at the polls. I test two social psychology theories affecting this process: self-prophecy and implementation intentions. The self-prophecy effect occurs wh...
متن کاملSame-Sex Marriage Ballot Measures and the 2004 Presidential Election
Did ballot measures banning samesex marriage swing the 2004 general election to George W. Bush? In 2004, activists and state legislators placed anti-gay marriage questions on the general election ballots of 11 states. All of the ballot measures passed easily, receiving on average roughly 70 percent support.1 Pundits argued that the marriage measures on the November ballot would be a major motiv...
متن کامل